Tonal Variations of Tripura (Usoi) Language (https://doi.org/10.63386/610875)
Md. Mostafa Rashel
Associate Professor of Linguistics, Department of English, Faculty of Humanities and Social Sciences, Daffodil International University
Abstract
Tripura, also known as Tripuri, is one of the major languages spoken in the state of Tripura, located in the North-East region of India and belongs to the Bodo-Garo group of the Tibeto-Burman (TB) branch of the Sino-Tibetan (ST) family. The tone has almost disappeared among the speakers of the Tripura (Usoi) language in Bangladesh, especially young speakers due to the influence of the majority of languages in Bangladesh like Bangla, Chakma, and Marma. The study highlights how tones operate in both monosyllabic and disyllabic words, with clearer contrasts observed in single-syllable words, and provides a brief description of tones of other TB languages. The research explains that there are three tones in the Tripura (Usoi) language: High tone and Falling tone are represented by an acute accent ( ́) and grave accent ( ̀) respectively while Mid tone is unmarked, and also identifies minimal sets of words differing only in tone, illustrating the functional role of tones in distinguishing meaning in Tripura language. The analysis of the Usoi variety suggests that most monosyllabic word roots carry a mid-tone. Overall, the study sheds light on the tone system of the Tripura (Usoi) language with a particular emphasis on the southern part of Chittagong Hill Tracts (CHT) in Bangladesh and provides valuable insights into its variations, functions, and implications for further research.
Keywords: Tibeto-Burman (TB), tones, frequency, triplets, monosyllabic, disyllabic
1. Introduction
Tripura language, also known as Tripuri, is one of the major languages of Tripura state in North-East India. Tripura belongs to the Bodo-Garo group of the TB branch of the ST family. Tripuri is also known as Kokborok (ISO 639-3 trp), which is also known as Tipperah, Tippurah, Tipra, Tipura, Triperah, and Tripura in Tripura state and is also spoken in some parts of Bangladesh, especially in the CHT. The word ‘Tripra’ is said to originate from the word’s tui ‘water’ and pra (which means confluence) in Tripura language. Therefore, the meaning is the people who live at the confluence of rivers (Tripura 1978: 4). These people were also known as Debbarmas, after the long-term rulers of this kingdom until it united with the Indian republic in 1949. Tripura is also the third largest population of the tribes in the CHT, Bangladesh. According to the 2022 Census of Bangladesh, the total number of Tripura in Bangladesh is about 156,578. Ethnically related to the Boro people of Assam, they refer to themselves as Borok or Twiprasa (Hoque, 2014: 34; Jamatia 1996: 1670). The researcher will use ‘Tripura’ to refer to the spoken language of Tripura people in Bandarban instead of ‘Kokborok’ throughout the article. In Bangladesh, Tripura people themselves call their language ‘Tripura’, ‘Tipera’, or ‘Tipra’. In addition, they learn Bangla, the principal language of Bangladesh. In Bangladesh, Tripura is spoken in Bandarban, Rangamati, and Khagrachari in CHT. ‘Kok’ in Tripura means ‘word’ or ‘language’ and ‘Borok’ means ‘people’. Thus, the literal meaning of Kokborok is the language spoken by the people (Bulbul 2000: 11; Baskaran 2015: 33; Jamatia 1996: 1670). Tripura is also recognized as a state language and the second official language of Tripura State in India and it is used in daily conversation at home and the marketplace in Bandarban, Rangamati, Khagrachari in Bangladesh, and Tripura State in India.
1.1 Linguistic Environment of Tripura Language
Long ago, Tripura State was predominantly Tripura-speaking, but numerous Bengalis have now settled there and the indigenous Tibeto-Burman speaking Tripuri, once the dominant group, have been reduced to a minority in their own homeland (Burling 2003: 169). Many Tripuris in Bangladesh and Tripura State are bilingual, or even multilingual, due to the influence of the dominant Bangla language (Baskaran 2015: 33), and Bengalis form a major part of the local population. Currently, some Bengalis who are living in Tripura can speak both Bangla and Tripura language (Jacquesson 2008: 6), but in general Bangla language is overpowering Tripura language. Today, apart from farmers, many Bangladeshi Tripura are traders, service-people, and professionals such as doctors, engineers, etc. When they move to urban centres, Tripura people do not use Tripura language. As with other linguistic minorities in Bangladesh, Tripura also endure a disadvantage as the medium of instruction in schools is Bangla. Tripura people do not currently use their own script to write their language. Koloma was the traditional script of Kokborok, which has now disappeared (Hasan 2011: 116). This Koloma script is based on the Bengali/Assamese. Tripura written language is traditionally said to have existed in its various forms since at least the 1st century AD, the traditional starting date of the historical records of Tripura Kings. The Tripura script is found on ancient coins of Tripura from the 16th century (Jacquesson 2008: 7). The history of Tripura includes two distinct periods- the largely legendary period described in the Rajamala, a chronicle of the supposed early kings of Tripura, and the period since the reign of the great king Dharma Manikya (reigned c. 1431-62). The ancient book Rajratnakar, also known as Rajmala, was originally written in Koloma script by Durlobendra Chantai. It is a chronicle of 184 Tripuri kings. Later, this book was translated into Sanskrit by the Brahmans, Sukreshwar and Vaneshwar in the court of Dharma Manikya (reigned c. 1463-1515) and later translated into Bangla language. The script is still sometimes used for traditional astrology.
2. Research Methodology
Preliminary steps include an extensive review of previous work on all varieties of the Tripura (Usoi) language. The researcher, for field data collection, had visited Bandarban, Rangamati, and Khagrachari, but the research work was primarily carried on in the southern part of the Chittagong Hill Tracts (CHT) because the speakers live mostly in this area. The researcher applied the language documentation approach and naturalistic data analysis for the tone study in the Tripura language. The methodology is conducting linguistic data collection from the samples of native speakers’ spoken texts, assembling a corpus of the samples, and representing it in the Tripura language. The data are systematically extracted to determine the patterns of tone and functions of the tone concerning different syllables. The comparative analysis of other Tibetan languages mainly those which lie in the Bodo-Garo group shed light on variations. The researcher used qualitative approaches like interviews, focus group discussions, an observational method, and documentary research, in addition to field methods such as audio and video recording, to analyze and distinguish the function and differences of tones across the linguistic varieties. This approach intends to realize comprehensive insights using linguistic aspects from the descriptive linguistics domain regarding the tonal issue in Tripura (Usoi) language. The study identifies three tones in the Tripura language: High tone, Mid-tone, and Falling tone. These tones are analyzed based on data collected from native speakers, with measurements of fundamental frequency (F0) and duration.
3. Previous Discussion on Tripura (Usoi) Tones
The research done on the Tripura language in India, such as Pai (1976: 17) and Jacquesson (2008: 6) collected data based on the speech of Agartala, Tripura in India. Pai mentioned that Kokborok has two tones: level tone and high tone, and Jacquesson (2008: 29) refers to high tone (ɟasku2 ‘knee’) and low tone (ɟasku ‘nail’). Both analyses suggest a contrast of two-level tones. This is unlike the most widespread pattern in a variety of Bodo-Garo languages, as in Bodo which has a level tone and a falling tone. Huziwara (2008: 198) collected data at the Rowangchari region in the Karnaphuli basin, Bandarban (CHT), Bangladesh, and found two distinctive tones: high tone and falling tone. Tripura in India is fully tonal, like many Bodo-Garo (BG) languages (but not Garo). It appears that Bangladesh Tripura is like Deori (Jacquesson 2005) which has marginal tonal distinctions in the speech of the oldest people but has lost tonal contrasts in the speech of all younger speakers. It also appears that the oldest speakers have a system different from that of Tripura in India and Bodo, among other BG languages, with a High tone, a Mid-tone, and a Falling tone. A similar analysis has also been proposed for Manipuri/Meithei (Chelliah 2003: 428; Singh & Singh 2007: 1), which is not a BG language but is spoken in the same area. Some BG systems (and another analysis of Manipuri/Meithei) have only two tones, one level and one falling. Karbi (Konnerth 2014: 76) discusses the issues involved in low functional load for tones, which means that tones are there but because the speakers do not rely on the tone differences as much as speakers of, for example, Thai or Mandarin, there may be variation in the way that speakers pronounce and feel about the tones. In the paper by Konnerth and Teoh (2014: 23), they showed that when playing back recordings of words where speakers clearly distinguished three tones, the listeners did not always perceive and distinguish them correctly. The tone has almost disappeared among the speakers of Tripura in Bangladesh, especially young speakers, probably due to the influence of the majority languages in Bangladesh like Bangla and Chakma (Huziwara 2003).
For the tone analysis, data from three older speakers is presented here (two males and one female). The recordings were processed and analyzed using Praat (Boersma and Weenink 2018) after the collection of the speech data. Each iteration of the target word was segmented based on information in the waveform and the spectrogram. The sound files were annotated in a Praat text grid. For getting the measurements (e.g., duration, F0 mean, onset and offset value, pitch range) F0 script was used. The speakers pronounced each word in isolation with three repetitions. Three tokens were used to allow for possible influences of prosody. These were recorded in audio and video mode. For identifying the tone, monosyllabic and disyllabic roots were used. The duration and fundamental frequency of each word were measured. From the data, it appears that three tones exist in Tripura. The ‘High’ tone is somewhat rising, ‘Mid’ tone is mid slightly falling, and ‘Falling’ tone is indeed falling sharply from fairly high.
The existing data shows that the pitch of all tones differs from speaker to speaker. Naturally, the pitch frequency is quite high for the female (speaker 3). Data from two speakers, both male, was used for the analysis of the Mid-tone. The female speaker did not have this tone in her speech; she used a Falling Tone in words where the male speakers have a Mid-tone. The values of High Tone, Mid Tone, and Falling Tone are given in Tables 4, 5, and 6. Table 1 shows the mean value of onset and offset F0 and duration of the High-tone first syllable for three speakers. For one male and one female speaker, there is a substantial rise in F0, for the other male speaker the rise is less substantial.
High Tone | Speaker | Onset | Offset | Duration | Rise of pitch |
Mean value | 1 (M) | 150 | 165 | 0.36 | 15 |
2 (M) | 93 | 96 | 0.29 | 3 | |
3 (F) | 195 | 253 | 0.58 | 58 |
Table 1 Mean fundamental frequency of onset and offset of High Tone (3 speakers)
Table 2 shows the mean value of onset and offset F0 and duration of the Mid-tone first syllable for two male speakers. This shows a very short fall.
Mid-tone | Speaker | Onset | Offset | Duration | Fall of pitch |
Mean value | 1 (M) | 156 | 154 | 0.42 | 2 |
2 (M) | 93 | 91 | 0.36 | 2 | |
3 (F) | – | – | – | – |
Table 2 Mean fundamental frequency of onset and offset of Mid Tone (2 speakers)
Table 3 shows the mean value of onset and offset F0 and duration of the Falling tone for three speakers. Here the fall is substantial.
Falling Tone | Speaker | Onset | Offset | Duration | Fall of pitch |
Mean value | 1 (M) | 162 | 134 | 0.36 | 28 |
2 (M) | 103 | 80 | 0.32 | 23 | |
3 (F) | 194 | 146 | 0.35 | 48 |
Table 3 Mean fundamental frequency of onset and offset of Falling Tone (3 speakers)
Graphs 1 and 2 show the F0 means for the two male speakers. Note that the Falling tone covers nearly the entire pitch range, from high to low. The three tones can be contrasted in minimal sets, for the oldest speakers, as shown in the Tonal Minimal Triplets section.
Graph 1 Tone position of Tripura (Speaker 1)
Graph 2 Tone position of Tripura (Speaker 2)
High Tone in Tripura morpheme with Gloss | Speaker 1 (M) | Speaker 2 (M) | Speaker 3 (F) | ||||||||||
On set | Off-set | Duration | Rise of pitch | On set | Off-set | Duration | Rise of pitch | On set | Off-set | Duration | Rise of pitch | ||
tʰó ‘drop of liquid’ | 163 | 169 | 0.17 | 6 | 140 | 108 | 0.18 | -32 | 224 | 318 | 0.44 | 94 | |
ká ‘cry’ | 167 | 170 | 0.16 | 3 | 122 | 87 | 0.19 | -35 | 184 | 280 | 0.47 | 96 | |
kʰú ‘bad smell’ | 162 | 163 | 0.28 | 1 | 147 | 123 | 0.19 | -24 | 224 | 342 | 0.46 | 118 | |
kʰí ‘stool, closet’ | 164 | 166 | 0.24 | 2 | 158 | 117 | 0.22 | -41 | 217 | 320 | 0.52 | 103 | |
dú ‘six’ | 165 | 187 | 0.3 | 22 | 129 | 110 | 0.2 | -19 | 172 | 129 | 0.23 | -43 | |
rí ‘scratch’, ‘draw’ | 155 | 177 | 0.39 | 22 | 104 | 120 | 0.23 | 16 | 188 | 132 | 0.52 | -56 | |
wá ‘bite’ | 158 | 169 | 0.33 | 11 | 95 | 97 | 0.16 | 2 | 173 | 237 | 0.42 | 64 | |
lṹ ‘fever’ | 154 | 210 | 0.66 | 56 | – | – | – | – | 194 | 175 | 0.32 | -19 | |
ró ‘brush/soft’ | 143 | 161 | 0.26 | 18 | 106 | 105 | 0.36 | -1 | 189 | 289 | 0.49 | 100 | |
hó.du ‘many’ | 140 | 155 | 0.52 | 15 | 108 | 101 | 0.18 | -7 | 188 | 166 | 0.23 | -22 | |
sán~sám ‘pain’ | 143 | 149 | 0.56 | 6 | 114 | 98 | 0.35 | -16 | 114 | 163 | 0.32 | 49 | |
tʰúŋ ‘deep’ | 155 | 160 | 0.47 | 5 | 126 | 129 | 0.38 | 3 | 129 | 350 | 0.33 | 221 | |
prói ‘soon’ | 143 | 170 | 0.19 | 27 | 142 | 102 | 0.19 | -40 | 126 | 305 | 0.4 | 179 | |
rwá ‘leech’ | 143 | 155 | 0.38 | 12 | 107 | 82 | 0.27 | -25 | 180 | 142 | 0.35 | -38 | |
pʰái ‘study’ | 154 | 171 | 0.24 | 17 | 104 | 103 | 0.29 | -1 | 208 | 162 | 0.23 | -46 | |
rái ‘cane’ | 147 | 212 | 0.37 | 65 | 98 | 86 | 0.23 | -12 | 209 | 318 | 0.58 | 109 | |
á.juŋ ‘uncle’ | 142 | 147 | 0.61 | 5 | 94 | 102 | 0.48 | 8 | – | – | – | – | |
tó.kja ‘cap’, ‘crown’ | 102 | 104 | 0.31 | 2 | 112 | 104 | 0.13 | -8 | 176 | 171 | 0.06 | -5 | |
bé ‘other’ | 194 | 131 | 0.34 | -63 | 79 | 83 | 0.51 | 4 | 196 | 143 | 0.36 | -53 | |
lá.ci ‘shame’ | 185 | 161 | 0.2 | -24 | 87 | 90 | 0.15 | 3 | 184 | 164 | 0.2 | -20 | |
bú.a ‘teeth’ | 162 | 147 | 0.38 | -15 | 93 | 96 | 0.3 | 3 | 184 | 131 | 0.41 | -53 | |
slái ‘than’ | 183 | 152 | 0.31 | -31 | 140 | 141 | 0.42 | 1 | 225 | 145 | 0.39 | -80 | |
kráiŋ ‘extreme fun’ | 198 | 143 | 0.48 | -55 | 102 | 105 | 0.33 | 3 | 180 | 293 | 0.56 | 113 | |
kró ‘curly hair’ | 181 | 166 | 0.32 | -15 | 89 | 90 | 0.13 | 1 | – | – | – | – | |
bró ‘man’ | 161 | 178 | 0.34 | 17 | 89 | 91 | 0.31 | 2 | 186 | 152 | 0.28 | -34 | |
krá ‘ripe/deep forest’ | 189 | 150 | 0.38 | -39 | 82 | 88 | 0.29 | 6 | 169 | 141 | 0.43 | -28 | |
brúŋ ‘Tripura nation’ | 169 | 171 | 0.47 | 2 | 81 | 84 | 0.23 | 3 | 182 | 142 | 0.45 | -40 | |
sá ‘tell’ | 169 | 140 | 0.3 | -29 | 114 | 95 | 0.22 | -19 | 196 | 243 | 0.43 | 47 | |
ɟú.mi ‘lean/decrease’ | 157 | 216 | 0.54 | 59 | 102 | 91 | 0.13 | -11 | 193 | 259 | 0.67 | 66 | |
rú.na ‘open yam’ | 185 | 137 | 0.51 | -48 | 103 | 92 | 0.48 | -11 | 181 | 260 | 0.7 | 79 | |
sú.na ‘making rice’ | 185 | 155 | 0.44 | -30 | 122 | 119 | 0.37 | -3 | 196 | 250 | 0.51 | 54 | |
só.na ‘wash’ | – | – | – | – | 125 | 136 | 0.35 | 11 | 211 | 256 | 0.6 | 45 |
Table 4 Three speakers’ production of the same morphemes in High Tone
Table 4 shows tokens of High Tone from all three speakers. Speakers 1 and 2 have overall higher F0 compared to speaker 3. The contour of this tone varies greatly for the female speaker 3, sometimes showing a large rise, but also sometimes showing a smaller fall. A minus sign in the ‘Rise of F0’ column in this table indicates that the F0 falls; similarly, a minus sign in the ‘Fall of F0’ column in Tables 6 and 7 indicates that the F0 rises. Note that there is some variation in the High Tone; it is sometimes replaced by a Mid or Falling Tone in some words by some speakers.
Mid Tone in Tripura morpheme with Gloss | Speaker 1 (M) | Speaker 2 (M) | Speaker 3 (F) | ||||||||||
On set | Offset | Duration | Fall of pitch | On set | Offset | Duration | Fall of pitch | On set | Offset | Duration | Fall of pitch | ||
ha ‘soil’ | 156 | 154 | 0.30 | 2 | 113 | 92 | 0.26 | 21 | 191 | 153 | 0.39 | 38 | |
ho ‘belly’ | 164 | 162 | 0.21 | 2 | 130 | 109 | 0.16 | 21 | 184 | 101 | 0.25 | 83 | |
bu ‘beat’ | 157 | 153 | 0.62 | 4 | 108 | 101 | 0.24 | 7 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | |
ci.ni ‘our’ | 174 | 170 | 0.39 | 4 | 116 | 101 | 0.38 | 15 | 196 | 139 | 0.55 | 57 | |
ma.do ‘cigarette’ | 145 | 144 | 0.53 | 1 | 111 | 123 | 0.21 | -12 | 180 | 151 | 0.39 | 29 | |
be.ra ‘keep’ | 143 | 143 | 0.49 | 0 | 106 | 104 | 0.37 | 2 | 189 | 157 | 0.57 | 32 | |
kʰnai ‘hair’ | 153 | 149 | 0.45 | 4 | 122 | 91 | 0.34 | 31 | 205 | 145 | 0.4 | 60 | |
kʰnu ‘show’ | 156 | 155 | 0.3 | 1 | 115 | 103 | 0.25 | 12 | 209 | 140 | 0.43 | 69 | |
la ‘take’ | 153 | 143 | 0.42 | 10 | 93 | 88 | 0.49 | 5 | 165 | 150 | 0.39 | 15 | |
ba.ri ‘small fence’ | 173 | 150 | 0.48 | 23 | 94 | 92 | 0.46 | 2 | 175 | 137 | 0.48 | 38 | |
bu.lai ‘fight’ | 160 | 133 | 0.53 | 27 | 95 | 94 | 0.29 | 1 | 195 | 156 | 0.53 | 39 | |
pʰo.ha ‘piece’ | 181 | 138 | 0.54 | 43 | 93 | 89 | 0.42 | 4 | 216 | 151 | 0.43 | 65 | |
toŋ ‘big rice pot’ | 203 | 160 | 0.34 | 43 | 107 | 105 | 0.3 | 2 | 189 | 152 | 0.39 | 37 | |
grai ‘kidney stone’ | 182 | 192 | 0.33 | -10 | 78 | 77 | 0.23 | 1 | 187 | 142 | 0.43 | 45 |
Table 5 Three speakers’ production of the same morphemes in Mid-tone
Table 5 shows the forms for which male speakers 1 and 2 have Mid Tone; note that female speaker 3 has Falling Tone for these forms. Thus, Speakers 1 and 2 have what appears to be an innovative system with a Mid Tone, while Speaker 3, the female speaker, has a conservative system similar to Tripura in India with a High Tone, sometimes rising, and a Falling Tone.
Falling Tone in Tripura morpheme with Gloss | Speaker- 3 (F) | Speaker-1 (M) | Speaker-2 (M) | |||||||||
Onset | Offset | Duration | Fall of pitch | Onset | Offset | Duration | Fall of pitch | Onset | Offset | Duration | Fall of pitch | |
sà ‘little’ | 188 | 155 | 0.27 | 33 | 174 | 147 | 0.24 | 27 | 123 | 109 | 0.22 | 14 |
sò ‘who’ | 194 | 141 | 0.3 | 53 | 172 | 131 | 0.27 | 41 | 131 | 96 | 0.29 | 35 |
kà ‘climb’ | 183 | 140 | 0.37 | 43 | 182 | 128 | 0.3 | 54 | 123 | 87 | 0.29 | 36 |
kʰì ‘stool’, ‘closet’ | 212 | 155 | 0.4 | 57 | 189 | 138 | 0.33 | 51 | 109 | 103 | 0.19 | 6 |
kʰì ‘treenail’, ‘pin’ | 208 | 146 | 0.33 | 62 | 193 | 139 | 0.32 | 54 | 124 | 99 | 0.24 | 25 |
tà ‘moon’ | 210 | 129 | 0.33 | 81 | 207 | 123 | 0.35 | 84 | 117 | 87 | 0.28 | 30 |
tʰù ‘lime’ | 202 | 152 | 0.4 | 50 | 186 | 138 | 0.37 | 48 | – | – | – | – |
hò ‘fire’ | 207 | 144 | 0.34 | 63 | 170 | 146 | 0.33 | 24 | 113 | 100 | 0.2 | 13 |
hò ‘night’ | 207 | 135 | 0.36 | 72 | 174 | 146 | 0.31 | 28 | 112 | 81 | 0.3 | 31 |
cà ‘eat’ | 160 | 131 | 0.26 | 29 | 193 | 155 | 0.33 | 38 | 123 | 99 | 0.23 | 24 |
ɟì ‘Kobra snake’ | 190 | 139 | 0.36 | 51 | 164 | 140 | 0.46 | 24 | – | – | – | – |
nù ‘see’ | 187 | 165 | 0.44 | 22 | 160 | 195 | 0.41 | +35 | 112 | 96 | 0.22 | 16 |
rì ‘cloth’ | 198 | 149 | 0.4 | 49 | 177 | 132 | 0.4 | 45 | 101 | 98 | 0.27 | 3 |
lã̀ ‘path, road’ | 142 | 114 | 0.44 | 28 | 178 | 129 | 0.45 | 49 | 117 | 87 | 0.38 | 30 |
kà.mi ‘village’ | 173 | 132 | 0.43 | 41 | 193 | 143 | 0.47 | 50 | 113 | 90 | 0.46 | 23 |
tòm ‘rice basket’ | 197 | 137 | 0.41 | 60 | 209 | 153 | 0.37 | 56 | – | – | – | – |
slài ‘arm’ | 208 | 149 | 0.28 | 59 | 181 | 138 | 0.38 | 43 | 128 | 100 | 0.31 | 28 |
klài ‘easy’, cheap’ | 186 | 125 | 0.39 | 61 | 188 | 148 | 0.33 | 40 | 113 | 87 | 0.38 | 26 |
klài ‘fall’ | 200 | 125 | 0.33 | 75 | 190 | 163 | 0.35 | 27 | 102 | 100 | 0.34 | 2 |
krã̀ ‘dry’ | 222 | 196 | 0.33 | 26 | 198 | 160 | 0.41 | 38 | 106 | 97 | 0.32 | 9 |
snùs ‘dirty’ | 250 | 234 | 0.23 | 16 | – | – | – | – | 110 | 94 | 0.29 | 16 |
sòi ‘dog’ | 195 | 143 | 0.39 | 52 | 184 | 157 | 0.36 | 27 | 110 | 99 | 0.27 | 11 |
sòi ‘write’ | 194 | 138 | 0.38 | 56 | 176 | 134 | 0.26 | 42 | 114 | 89 | 0.31 | 25 |
tʰòi ‘blood’ | 185 | 135 | 0.34 | 50 | 181 | 149 | 0.35 | 32 | 125 | 102 | 0.25 | 23 |
pài ‘finish’ | 187 | 163 | 0.49 | 24 | 182 | 163 | 0.36 | 19 | 109 | 99 | 0.27 | 10 |
pài ‘buy’ | 176 | 162 | 0.45 | 14 | 187 | 141 | 0.36 | 46 | 116 | 94 | 0.22 | 22 |
Table 6 Three speakers’ production of the same morphemes in Falling tone.
Table 6 shows the forms with Falling Tones for all three speakers. As for the other tones, male speaker 2 uses an overall lower F0 range. All three speakers show a substantial fall in F0 for most forms.
There is some variation in the distribution of tones between speakers. For example, in some instances, both male speakers have a High Tone, but the female speaker has a Falling Tone; in a few tokens where two of the three speakers have a Falling Tone, the third speaker has a High Tone; for example, male speaker 1 appears to have a High Tone nú for ‘see’, while the other two have a Falling Tone nù.
This study has identified that for the male speakers that we recorded, a system of three distinct tones can be proposed. However, the variation discussed here suggests that more research is needed to fully understand the system.
4.1 Tonal Minimal Triplets
/á/ vs. /à/ vs. /a/
lài ‘rice pots’
lái ‘banana leaves’
bisà ‘poison’
bisa ‘fly’
wà ‘bamboo’
wá ‘bite’
klài ‘easy’, ‘cheap’
klai ‘must’
kà ‘climb’, ‘run’
ká ‘cry’, ‘arise’
ka ‘footsteps’, ‘promote’
krá ‘ripe’
krà ‘hard’, ‘father-in-law’
sà ‘sun’, ‘day’
sa ‘please-politeness’, ‘one’
sá ‘tell’
sài ‘thousand’
sai ‘army officer’
rwá ‘leech
rwà ‘axe’
lṹ ‘fever’
lã̀ ‘road’
la ‘take’
/é/ vs /è/ vs /e/
rèiŋ ‘call’
réiŋ ‘prompt’
sè ‘scales’
se ‘only/animate marker’
tʰè ‘small dam’
tʰe ‘clf. fruits’
/í/ vs. /ì/ vs/ i/
kʰì ‘trenail’, ‘nail’
kʰí ‘stool’, ‘closet’
rí ‘scratch’
rì ‘cloth’
ri ‘look after’
/ó/ vs. /ò/ vs. /o/
tóŋ ‘carrying rice’
toŋ ‘completed’
ró ‘brush/soft’
rò ‘here’
hò ‘fire’, ‘night’
ho ‘belly’
sò ‘who’, ‘iron’
so ‘pull’
tòi ‘bring’, ‘water’, ‘egg’
toi ‘clf. egg’
/ú/ vs. /ù/ vs. /u/
túŋ ‘roof’
tùŋ ‘warm’
lṹ ‘fever’
lù ‘carry’, ‘sick’
nù ‘see’
nu ‘drink’
bú ‘mix’
bu ‘beat’, ‘snap’
bù ‘body swelling’
kʰnu ‘show’
kʰnù ‘hello’
ku.tʰùŋ ‘deep’
ku.tʰuŋ ‘green’
/ə́/ vs /ə̀/ vs /ə/
kə́.cà ‘half’
kə̀.cà ‘old/tight/second’
kə.cà ‘middle’
4.2 Tone in Monosyllabic and Disyllabic Words
In single-syllable words, the contrast among rising, falling, and mid tones is clearer than in words with two or more syllables. Mostly, one tone can occur per word. There can be two tones in some disyllabic or trisyllabic compound words in Tripura as in hó.tu ‘paper balloon’, tòi.kù.na ‘bath’, hà.pʰa.toŋ ‘residence’. Table 7 shows combinations of initial consonants, vowels, and High vs. Falling tones.
The most frequent combinations of vowel and tone of Tripura are /á, à, ì, ò, ú, ù/. Initial /k/ can combine with sesquisyllabic /ə/ with High and Falling tones, and /m/ only allows /ə̀/. The stop palatal /c/ does not combine with High tone.
á | à | é | è | í | ì | ó | ò | ú | ù | ə́ | ə̀ | |
p | – | √ | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – |
t | √ | √ | – | – | – | √ | √ | √ | – | √ | – | – |
c | – | √ | – | √ | – | √ | – | – | – | √ | – | – |
k | √ | √ | – | √ | – | √ | – | – | √ | – | √ | √ |
pʰ | √ | √ | – | – | – | – | – | √ | – | – | – | – |
tʰ | – | √ | – | √ | – | – | √ | √ | √ | √ | – | – |
kʰ | √ | – | – | – | √ | √ | – | √ | √ | – | – | – |
d | – | √ | – | – | – | – | – | – | √ | – | – | – |
b | √ | – | – | √ | – | – | – | – | √ | √ | – | – |
ɟ | √ | – | – | – | – | √ | – | – | √ | – | – | – |
g | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | √ | – | – | – | – |
s | √ | √ | – | √ | √ | √ | – | √ | √ | √ | – | – |
h | √ | √ | – | – | – | – | √ | √ | √ | – | – | – |
m | – | √ | – | – | √ | – | √ | – | – | – | – | √ |
n | √ | √ | – | – | – | √ | – | √ | √ | √ | – | – |
ŋ | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – |
r | √ | √ | – | – | √ | √ | √ | √ | √ | √ | – | – |
l | √ | √ | – | √ | √ | √ | – | – | √ | √ | – | – |
j | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – |
w | √ | √ | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – |
Table 7 Initial Consonant and Tone
Table 8 shows the relatively few tone contrasts after initial consonant clusters. The /ə/ with a High or Falling tone and /í/ with a High tone do not occur after any consonant clusters. /è, ì, ò/ occur after spl-, sn-, and kr- respectively while /a/ with High tone occurs only after cluster rw-. Diphthongs after an initial consonant cluster also have some tonal restrictions in Tripura. For instance, /ái/ only occurs after kr–, /òi/ after st-, /ói/ after pr, and /ào/ after kl–. Diphthong /ei/ with High or Falling tone and /áo/ with High tone does not occur after any cluster. Examples are given in the data tables.
á | à | é | è | ì | ó | ò | ú | ù | ái | ài | ói | òi | ào | |
pr– | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | √ | – | – |
cr– | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | √ | – | – | – |
kl– | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | √ | – | – | – | √ |
kr– | √ | √ | – | – | – | √ | √ | – | – | √ | – | – | – | – |
kʰn– | √ | – | – | – | – | – | – | √ | – | – | – | – | – | |
br– | – | – | – | – | – | √ | – | √ | √ | – | – | – | – | – |
bl– | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | √ | – | – | – | – |
st– | – | – | √ | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | √ | – |
sn– | – | – | – | – | √ | – | – | – | √ | – | – | – | – | – |
sl– | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | √ | √ | – | – | – |
spl– | – | – | – | √ | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | |
rw– | √ | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – |
Table 8 Consonant cluster plus tone (diphthongs)
For tone sequences in two-syllable words, data from the first speaker is presented here. The duration of the two syllables is shown separately in Tables 9 to 13. Where words contain two tones, the sequence can be HF, HM, FH, FF or MF. Table 9 shows the HF data.
High-Falling (HF) | Syllable | On set | Offset | Duration | Change of pitch |
sí.kàŋ ‘hip’ | sí | 178 | 183 | 0.11 | 5 |
kàŋ | 175 | 166 | 0.30 | 9 | |
mú.sù ‘cow’ | mú | 160 | 164 | 0.08 | 4 |
sù | 171 | 162 | 0.25 | 9 | |
kṹ.tài ‘shirt’ | kṹ | 161 | 163 | 0.20 | 2 |
tài | 182 | 144 | 0.21 | 38 |
The HM pattern is shown in Table 10.
High-Mid (HM) | Syllable | On set | Offset | Duration | Change of pitch |
kə́.cà ‘half’ | kə́ | 156 | 167 | 0.09 | 11 |
cà | 174 | 171 | 0.04 | 3 | |
lí.lì ‘about to die’ | lí | 154 | 170 | 0.29 | 16 |
lì | 160 | 159 | 0.29 | 1 | |
kʰá.kʰnú ‘pumpkin’ | kʰá | 142 | 149 | 0.04 | 7 |
kʰnú | 155 | 157 | 0.27 | 2 |
The FH pattern is shown in Table 11.
Falling-High (FH) | Syllable | On set | Offset | Duration | Change of pitch |
mà.ɟrú ‘to add’ | mà | 157 | 151 | 0.25 | 6 |
ɟrú | 144 | 154 | 0.37 | 10 | |
bì.sí ‘wife’ | bì | 136 | 133 | 0.05 | 3 |
sí | 159 | 164 | 0.21 | 5 | |
kài.skú ‘nine’ | kài | 164 | 108 | 0.23 | 56 |
skú | 156 | 217 | 0.13 | 61 | |
kè.sí ‘hand fan’ | kè | 173 | 163 | 0.05 | 10 |
sí | 175 | 179 | 0.21 | 4 |
The FF pattern is shown in Table 12. In general, the second Falling tone falls more.
Falling-Falling (FF) | Syllable | On set | Offset | Duration | Change of pitch |
sì.gò ‘neckless’ | sì | 174 | 155 | 0.15 | 19 |
gò | 157 | 134 | 0.17 | 23 | |
rì.tào ‘weave’ | rì | 156 | 146 | 0.42 | 10 |
tào | 160 | 136 | 0.17 | 24 | |
hàŋ.gò ‘coal’ | hàŋ | 161 | 146 | 0.24 | 15 |
gò | 149 | 134 | 0.25 | 15 | |
tlù.tlù ‘slowly’ | tlù | 169 | 152 | 0.29 | 17 |
tlù | 167 | 132 | 0.31 | 35 | |
kə̀.cà ‘old/sick’ | kə̀ | 167 | 160 | 0.11 | 7 |
cà | 177 | 146 | 0.12 | 31 | |
kì.sì ‘wet’ | kì | 177 | 166 | 0.07 | 11 |
sì | 157 | 137 | 0.23 | 20 | |
kài.cà ‘eight’ | kài | 171 | 128 | 0.29 | 43 |
cà | 164 | 152 | 0.17 | 12 | |
kè.sì ‘fan’ | kè | 162 | 157 | 0.04 | 5 |
sì | 171 | 161 | 0.16 | 10 |
The MF pattern is seen in Table 13.
Mid-Falling (MF) | Syllable | On set | Offset | Duration | Change of pitch |
mu.sù ‘spines’ | mu | 166 | 164 | 0.11 | 2 |
sù | 171 | 164 | 0.28 | 7 | |
u.tùŋ ‘after/behind’ | u | 165 | 163 | 0.26 | 2 |
tùŋ | 169 | 137 | 0.21 | 32 |
Therefore, the tones of Tripura are in the process of disappearing. Even for the three older speakers whose elicited careful speech data is discussed here, there are substantial interspeaker differences; two speakers have three tones, one has two, and all vary somewhat in their use. Younger speakers do not produce these tonal differences in their speech. Even older speakers often show variation between tones in the same word in similar situations. In examples in this paper where older speakers use a tone consistently, it is indicated with acute for High tone and grave for Falling tone.
5. Conclusions
In conclusion, for the oldest speakers, Tripura has three tones (High, Mid, and Falling) in Bangladesh; two tones (High and Falling) contrast in Tripura in India. The Mid-tone is a new development in Tripura in Bangladesh. It perhaps is a stage on the way to the loss of tones, with the High and Falling tones sometimes neutralising to Mid-tone in some words for some speakers. The female speaker whose data for High and Falling tones is presented in this paper does not have the Mid-tone. Therefore, the tone system of Usoi is an area where further research would be useful, particularly concerning the Mid-tone which is not reported in Tripura as spoken in India.
Acknowledgements: I collected these data during my PhD fieldwork and would like to express my gratitude to La Trobe University for awarding me the La Trobe University Postgraduate Research and Full Free Scholarships. These scholarships enabled me to conduct research on this language.
References
Bangladesh Population Census (2011). Dhaka: BangladeshPopulation Census.
Baskaran, S. G. (2015). Classifiers in Kokborok. International Journal of Linguistics and
Literature, 4(5): 33-42.
Boersma, P. & Weenink, D. (2018). Praat: doing phonetics by computer. Version 6.0.37.
Netherlands: Amsterdam University. Retrieved from http://www.praat.org/.
Bulbul, A. (2000). Parbotto Chottogramer Tripura Bhasa Poriciti (Introducing Tripura
Language of Chittagong Hill Tracts). Dhaka: Agami Publishers.
Burling, R. (2003). The Tibeto-Burman Languages of Northeastern India. In G. Thurgood &
R. J. LaPolla (eds), The Sino-Tibetan Languages, 169-191. London & New York: Routledge.
Chelliah, S. L. (2003). Meithei. In G. Thurgood & R. J. LaPolla (eds), The Sino-Tibetan
Languages, 427-438. London & New York: Routledge.
Hasan, M. K. (2011). Causative Constructions in Kok-Borok. Dhaka University Journal of Linguistics 2(4): 115-
137.
Hoque, F. (2014). Kokborok: A Major Tribal Language of Tripura. IOSR Journal of
Humanities And Social Science (IOSR-JHSS) 19 (10, V): 30-35.
Huziwara, K. (2003). Tone Sandhi in Chakma and Cak. Paper presented at the 36th
International Conference on Sino-Tibetan Languages and Linguistics, La Trobe University.
Huziwara, K. (2008). Notes on Usoi Tripura Phonetics and Phonology. In G. Hyslop, S. Morey
& M. W. Post (eds) Northeast Indian Linguistics 4: 197-215. New Delhi: Cambridge University Press.
Jacquesson, F. (2005). Le deuri: langue tibéto-birmane d’Assam. Leuven & Dudley, MA:
Peeters.
Jacquesson, F. (2008). A Kokborok Grammar (Agartala dialect). Agartala, Tripura: Kokborok
Tei Hukumu Mission.
Jamatia, N. (1996). Kokborok is One of the Rich Languages of the World. Paper presented at
the 4th International Symposium on Language and Linguistics, Bangkok.
Konnerth, L. A. (2014). A Grammar of Karbi. (PhD), University of Oregon, Oregon. Retrieved
from https://scholarsbank.uoregon.edu/xmlui/handle/1794/17928.
Konnerth, L. A. & Teoh, A. (2014). Acoustic-statistical and perceptual investigations of Karbi
tones: A peculiar case of incomplete neutralization of F0. In G. Hyslop, L. Konnerth, S. Morey & P. Sarmah (eds) North Easy Indian Linguistics 6. Pacific Linguistics A-PL 014: 13-38.
Pai, P. (1976). Kokborok Grammar. Mysore, India: Central Institute of Indian Languages.
Singh, H. D. & Singh, S. I. (2007). Exotic Phonemes: A Study of Manipuri Phonemes.
Language in India 7(13).
Tripura, B. (1978). The Tripuras of Chittagong Hill Tracts. Chittagong: Tripura Upajati
Shikha-O-Sanskritik Sangsad.